Friston et al. (2000) suggested that current techniques for estimating the
autocorrelation (Autoregressive(AR) and models where
is the frequency)
are not accurate enough to give prewhitening
acceptable bias.
Therefore, estimation is made more robust to inaccurate autocorrelation estimates
by swamping any intrinsic
autocorrelation with the known autocorrelation introduced by band
pass filtering, an approach often referred to as ``colouring'' (Friston et al., 1995), (Worsley and Friston, 1995).
For this they use a Gaussian (or similar) low-pass filter matched to the
haemodynamic response function (HRF) and a linear high-pass filter
which aims to remove the majority of the autocorrelation
due to low frequency components.
Having shaped the autocorrelation, prewhitening is then not applicable,
and the autocorrelation estimate is instead used to
correct the variance of univariate linear model
parameter estimates and the degrees of
freedom used in the GLM.
Although colouring is unbiased, given an accurate autocorrelation estimate, Bullmore et al. (1996) noted the need for serially independent (whitened) residuals to obtain the Best Linear Unbiased Estimates (BLUE) of the GLM parameters. The parameter estimates are ``best'' in the sense that they are the unbiased estimates with the lowest variance. This was achieved using Pseudo-Generalised Least Squares (PGLS) - also known as the Cochrane-Orcutt transformation. The autocorrelation is estimated for the residuals from a first linear model and is then used to ``prewhiten'' the data and the design matrix, for use in a second linear model. The residuals of the second linear model should be close to white noise. Further iterations of this process are possible.
For inference, they ascertain the null distributions using randomisation; that is they randomly reorganise the order of signal intensity values in each observed time series and estimate the test statistic for each. It is important to note that such randomisation of the time series is only valid in the absence of autocorrelations, hence an accurate prewhitening step is necessary. Any randomisation approach on non-white data needs to randomise the data in such a way that the effective structure of the autocorrelation is maintained, for the null distribution to be valid.
To model the autocorrelation, Bullmore proposed an AR model
of order 1 (AR(1)), which was shown to model the autocorrelations
satisfactorily for the data used in their paper.
Purdon and Weisskoff (1998) also suggested using an AR(1) model
to do prewhitening, but they also included a
white noise component. However, the main focus of their
paper was to explore the effect, on the false positive rate, of not taking
into account the temporal autocorrelation. For a desired
false positive rate of
they find false positive
rates as high as
in the uncorrected data.
At
the situation worsens further, with uncorrected data
giving
. This is because any inaccuracies in the
distribution compared with the assumed theoretical distribution
are more prominent further down the tail of the distribution.
Locascio et al. (1997) used an Autoregressive Moving Average (ARMA) model (see also Chatfield (1996)) and incorporated it into an overall Contrast Autoregressive and Moving Average (CARMA) model. As well as the ARMA and modelled experimental responses, the CARMA model contains baseline, linear and quadratic terms for the removal of low frequency drift. They fit separate MA and AR models of up to order 3.
Locascio et al. (1997) also suggests that the existence of positive autocorrelation is due to carry over from one time point to the next, stemming from time intervals that are smaller than the actual temporal changes. They describe autocorrelation as the persistence of neuronal activation, cyclical events (presumably they are referring to aliased cardiac and respiratory cycles), or possibly characteristics or artefacts of the measurement process.
Zarahn et al. (1997) and Aguirre et al. (1997) observed
noise profiles in FMRI data, and as a result attempted to use a
noise model with three parameters to account for temporal
autocorrelations. They also carried out a number of water phantom studies to
establish how much, if any, of the
noise is attributable to
physiological processes. They concluded that the same
noise
was apparent in the phantoms and that therefore the noise was not
of physiological origin.
They use their model of the intrinsic autocorrelation together
with Worsley and Friston (1995)'s
approach of colouring the data. Zarahn proposed to refine this
by incorporating the
model's representation of the intrinsic
autocorrelation into the autocorrelation due to temporal
filtering, in order to give a better estimate of the autocorrelation
post-temporal filtering. Unfortunately, their attempts fail
because they fit the
model over the entire brain volume,
therefore ignoring the potential for spatial non-stationarity of the noise
profile.
Hu et al. (1995) concentrate on the components of the coloured noise in FMRI data that are due to physiological fluctuations. By recording respiration and cardiac cycle data at the same time as the FMRI data is acquired, some of these effects can be removed. This could help to reduce the autocorrelation and potentially improve any autocorrelation estimation subsequently employed. However, these are not the only causes of coloured noise in the data and hence whether or not respiration and cardiac cycle data is available, robust strategies for dealing with autocorrelation are still required.