next up previous
Next: Discussion Up: Results Previous: Accuracy and dimensionality

Real FMRI data

For the first example, we used data courtesy of Dr. Dave McGonigle that previously had been used to evaluate the between-session variability in FMRI [McGonigle et al., 2000]. In brief, the experiment involved 33 sessions of runs under motor, cognitive and visual stimulation. The data presented here is one of the two visual stimulation sessions of 36 volumes each that proved unacceptably difficult to analyse using a model based approach and had therefore been excluded from the previous analysis due to obvious motion artifacts. It is used here to illustrate the advantages of model-free data analysis techniques in cases where the data does not conform to simple a priori hypotheses.


Figure 8: Analysis of visual stimulation data: (i) map from a fixed effects analysis of the non-motion confounded 31 data sets for reference, (ii) FEAT $ Z$-statistical maps ( $ Z>3.0, p<0.01$) obtained from GLM fitting of the motion-confounded data (left) and after the inclusion of estimated motion parameters as additional regressors of no interest (right), (iii) estimated motion parameters on this one data set that show a high absolute correlation with the stimulus, (iv) spatial maps from PICA performed in a space spanned by the 7 dominant eigenvectors, (v) set of spatial maps from a standard ICA analysis where the data was projected into a 29 (out of a possible 35) dimensional subspace of the data that retains $ >90\%$ of the overall variability in the data. For ICA and PICA all maps are shown where the associated time course has its peak power at the frequency of the stimulus.


Figure 8(i) show the results from a fixed effects analysis over the 31 non-confounded data sets after each set was analysed separately using FEAT. It shows the general visual activation pattern that emerged from the analysis of sessions that were not heavily confounded by subject motion. In contrast, figure 8(ii) shows sagittal maximum intensity projections of $ Z$ score maps from a GLM regression of one of the confounded data sets against the expected response. There are large amounts of non-plausible and 'spurious' activation. These results were obtained after initial rigid-body motion correction using MCFLIRT. Visual inspection of the data after correction suggested that the algorithm was able to realign the volumes reasonably well with no 'noticable' misalignment of neighbouring volumes. The estimated motion parameters in figure 8(iii) suggest that the poor localisation of visual cortical areas in the $ Z$ maps is not due to high magnitude of motion but instead is a result of a strong correlation between certain motion parameters and the stimulus sequence (stimulus correlated motion). Within the GLM framework, the classical approach is to include the estimated motion parameters as nuisance regressors. In this case, however, the GLM results do not improve and still do not uniquely identify visual cortical areas (figure 8(ii), second map).

In the case of a PICA analysis of the motion confounded data set, only seven component maps remained after dimensionality reduction of which only 2 maps have an associated time course where the highest power is at the frequency of stimulus presentation (figure 8(iv)). The results from a probabilistic independent component analysis clearly improve upon the GLM results in that the first PICA map shows a clean and well localised area of activation within the visual cortex similar to the area identified by the fixed effects analysis while the second map has large values at the intensity boundraries of the original EPI data and has an associated time course with high correlation to the estimated rotation around the Z-axis (iii, top). In comparison, figure 8(v) shows the result of a standard ICA decomposition, where the data was projected onto the dominant 29 eigenvectors in order to retain $ >90\%$ of the variability in the data. Using the same criterion for the selection of maps as before, seven components emerge (here ordered with decreasing absolute correlation from left to right after thresholding by converting each intensity value into a $ Z$ score and only retaining voxels with $ Z>2.3$). It is difficult to assess the differences between figure 8(iv) and (v) with respect to estimated motion. For the visual activation, however, the comparison suggests that results from classical ICA do actually overfit the data in that different features that appear both in the PICA map and fixed effects map are distributed across different spatial maps.


Figure 9: GLM vs. PICA on visual stimulation data: (i) FEAT results and regressor time course, (ii) Eigenspectrum of the data covariance matrix and estimate of the latent dimensionality using equation 10, (iii) & (iv) spatial maps and associated time courses of PICA results, all maps with $ r>0.3$ between the estimated and expected time course are shown.

As a second example, figure 9 shows the PICA results on the visual stimulation study used within the introduction to illustrate the problem of overfitting. Based on the estimate of the model order, the data was projected onto the first 27 eigenvectors prior to the unmixing. Comparing figure 9(i) and (ii) we get a much better correspondence between the areas of activation estimated from the GLM approach and the main PICA estimate (compared to figure 1(ii)). This is reassuring, since simple visual experiments of this kind are known to activate large visual cortical areas which should be reliably identifiable over a whole range of analysis techniques. Within the set of IC maps a second source estimate has an associated time course that correlates with the assumed response at $ r>0.3$. This map depicts a bilateral pattern of activation within visual cortical areas, possibly V3/MT, areas known to be involved in the processing of visual motion. This is highly plausible given that under the stimulation condition the volunteer was presented with a checkerboard reversing at 8Hz. The associated time course is very similar to the time course associated with the spatial map (iv) in figure 1, but in the case of standard ICA, only a unilateral activation is identified. This is not attributable to the difference in the thresholding itself; the raw IC map in figure 1 does not allow for a bilateral activation pattern. Instead, it turns out to be direct consequence of the existence of a noise model: the standard deviation of the residual noise in the PICA decomposition is comparably small within these areas. After transforming the raw IC estimates $ s$$ _i$ into $ Z$-scores, the well localised areas emerge. In addition, figure 10 shows a selection of maps found during the same PICA decomposition on this data, depicting e.g. physiological 'noise', motion and scanner artefacts. Note that in both examples the PICA maps are actual $ Z$ statistical maps and as such are much easier to compare against output from a standard GLM analysis. Standard ICA maps, for reasons outlined above, are simply raw parameter estimates and as such purely descriptive.


Figure 10: Additional PICA maps from the visual activation data: (i) head motion (translation in Z), (ii) sensory motor activation, (iii) signal fluctuations in areas close to the sinuses (possibly due to interaction of $ B_0$ field inhomogeneity with head motion), (iv) high frequency MR 'ghost' and (v) 'resting- state' fluctuations/ physiological noise.


next up previous
Next: Discussion Up: Results Previous: Accuracy and dimensionality
Christian F. Beckmann 2003-08-05